This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

Neuroscientists investigate how the target of an arm movement is spatially encoded in the primate brain

Neuroscientists investigate how the target of an arm movement is spatially encoded in the primate brain
Predominant object-centered encoding across neuronal populations in the size invariant task. a) Hypothetical and actual confusion matrices in cross-conditional decoding. b) Across the population of PMd (red) and PRR (blue) neurons, Position and Size Invariance values were mostly positive, indicating predominant size-invariant neural selectivity in the last 300 ms of visual memory period. c) Equivalently, positive RFD values indicate predominant classification of object-centered rather than egocentric positions, respectively. Credit: Nature Communications (2024). DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-47554-4

Saturday evening at a well-attended stand-up party. Servers balance trays of appetizers over the heads of the guests. We spot donuts on one of these passing platters and decide in a matter of seconds in favor of the one on the far right. But then the waitress briefly disappears from our field of vision, only to reappear elsewhere. How does our arm know where to reach to grab the donut?

Relative to our body, our favorite donut has changed its position in the meantime. However, relative to the tray on which the donut is lying, its position remains constant. Can our brain utilize this fact? How and where does our brain process the different spatial information in order to be able to successfully carry out targeted movements, such as reaching for the donut?

If we imagine a spatial coordinate system, its origin or zero point can be either body-related (direction of gaze, position of the hand, arm, center of the body) or object-related (position, size or orientation of an object in space). So far, it had been assumed that for directing our hand towards the favorite donut, only body-related reference systems are processed in different regions of the brain than object-related ones and that only body-related reference systems play a role in the planning of arm movements.

A study by the Sensorimotor Research Group at the German Primate Center, now published in Nature Communications, shows that this classic model needs to be re-evaluated.

In order to find out which coordinate system underlies the planning of goal-oriented movements, they trained two to memorize movement goals on a in order to reach for them later. The monkeys first saw an object consisting of five small squares next to each other on the screen, which were connected horizontally by a line (the "tray" in the example above).

In the next step, the squares disappeared and the monkeys saw a target dot positioned where one of the squares was previously visible, for example in the second square from the right (the position of the preferred donut). The dot then disappeared and after a short pause the five squares reappeared, but shifted a little to the right on the screen.

The monkeys were now asked to touch the square where they had previously seen the target dot, i.e., the second square from the right, although it was further to the right on the screen than before. While the monkeys performed the task, the activity of their nerve cells in the areas of the brain responsible for planning and executing targeted arm movements was measured.

"The results of the experiment surprised us," says Bahareh Taghizadeh, first author of the study. "Contrary to expectations, we found that both body- and object-related spatial reference systems can be detected in one and the same brain area and that these are also processed by the same nerve cells, dynamically adaptable depending on the cognitive requirement." The researchers found with such flexible spatial coding both in the parietal and the frontal lobe of the brain.

For the donut tray, this means that as long as our brain only processes the information that we want the right donut, this happens in an object-related coordinate system, i.e., relative to the tray, regardless of our own position in space or our line of sight. However, from the moment we actually plan to reach for the pastry, its position is coded relative to our own body.

The results are important for our basic understanding of brain function, but also for the development of neuroprostheses. The activity of the determines for the motor prosthesis to which target a movement is planned.

"If a is to be controlled, the information relative to which position in space the arm is to be moved upwards to the right, for example, is essential," explains Alexander Gail, head of the Sensorimotor Research Group and the study. "The correct control depends largely on describing the movement in the correct coordinate system."

Future studies should clarify whether and to what extent in these brain regions coordinate systems that are defined by the surrounding space also play a role in planning movements when oneself moves through the room.

More information: Bahareh Taghizadeh et al, Position- and scale-invariant object-centered spatial localization in monkey frontoparietal cortex dynamically adapts to cognitive demand, Nature Communications (2024). DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-47554-4

Journal information: Nature Communications
Citation: Neuroscientists investigate how the target of an arm movement is spatially encoded in the primate brain (2024, April 26) retrieved 8 May 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2024-04-neuroscientists-arm-movement-spatially-encoded.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Model describes complete grasping movement planning in the brain

29 shares

Feedback to editors