Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder marked by impairments in social communication as well as restricted, repetitive patterns of interests, behaviors, and activities1. ASD is highly heritable and may result from a number of genetic anomalies, including chromosomal copy number variation, single gene mutations, epigenetic changes, and complex inheritance2,3,4,5. Despite this knowledge, only 10–20% of ASD cases have an identified etiology6. As such, most research to date has been conducted on cases with “idiopathic” ASD samples with marked phenotypic and genetic heterogeneity. As a result, it has been difficult to identify therapeutic targets in ASD. One way to surmount this challenge is to study ASD through the lens of specific genetic causes. This approach may serve to distill the complexity and heterogeneity of idiopathic ASD and address the possibility that seemingly unrelated genetic disorders associated with ASD may converge on final common pathways7,8. Therefore, better understanding of mechanistic commonalities among distinct causes may shed light on the pathogenesis of ASD, opening the door to potential targeted treatments9,10.

PTEN hamartoma tumor syndrome (PHTS) is one example of a genetic disorder associated with a relatively high prevalence of ASD. PHTS is caused by germline mutations in PTEN, which encodes phosphatase and tensin homolog, resulting in a multitude of presentations that range from increased cancer risks to macrocephaly and neurodevelopmental impairment11,12,13. By some estimates, the prevalence of ASD in PHTS is around 22%14, and studies suggest that up to 20% of individuals with ASD with macrocephaly may have a pathogenic PTEN variant15,16,17,18.

Compared to other ASD groups (i.e., macrocephalic ASD, normocephalic ASD) and healthy controls, individuals with ASD and PHTS appear to have a specific pattern of increased and poorly-developed white matter with deficits in processing speed and working memory that extend beyond overall reductions in intellectual functioning19. While there is evidence to suggest that patients with PHTS without ASD also have a pattern of cognitive performance that implicates the involvement of frontal lobe systems20, these patient groups have never been directly compared across a comprehensive neuropsychological battery to differentiate PTEN-specific and ASD-specific deficits. Furthermore, we are unaware of any studies that have comprehensively examined behavioral phenotypes in these patient groups.

The goal of the current study was to comprehensively characterize, cross-sectionally, the neurobehavioral phenotype of PTEN-ASD in a large, prospective cohort and to examine phenotypic differences between these individuals and two groups of controls (i.e., ASD with macrocephaly but without PTEN mutation [Macro-ASD], PTEN mutation without ASD [PTEN-no ASD]). Based on preliminary data in independent samples19,20, we hypothesized that: (1) PTEN-ASD would have lower cognitive scores than the other patient groups, particularly on measures of intelligence, processing speed, working memory, and motor functioning; (2) PTEN-ASD would have comparable autism symptom levels to Macro-ASD, but lower intelligence (due to differences in processing speed and/or working memory) and adaptive function; and (3) PTEN-no ASD would demonstrate reduced performance on measures sensitive to frontal lobe functioning compared to healthy control norms.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants in this study were recruited from four large tertiary medical centers (Cleveland Clinic, Boston Children’s Hospital, Stanford University Medical Center, and University of California, Los Angeles) as part of an IRB-approved, ongoing, multicenter prospective study designed to examine the natural history of autism and germline heterozygous PTEN mutations (clinicaltrials.gov: NCT02461446). All potential study participants were screened by a clinical psychologist with expertise in ASD to determine if they met DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for ASD. All potential participants also underwent genetic testing to determine the presence/absence of a mutation in PTEN. Individuals were included in the study if they met the following criteria: (1) age 3–21 years; (2) confirmed diagnosis of ASD (based on consensus of expert clinician evaluation and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2) and/or a confirmed heterozygous mutation in PTEN; (3) English as primary communicative language; and (4) completion of baseline neuropsychological evaluation. In addition, individuals with ASD, but without a PTEN mutation, had to have a occipitofrontal head circumference ≥98th percentile to be included in the study. Informed consent for study participation was obtained from adult participants and/or a parent or legal guardian. Assent was obtained from all participants age 7 years and older who were cognitively able to provide same.

A total of 84 participants met study inclusion criteria and were categorized into one of the following study groups based on clinical diagnosis and results of PTEN genotyping: PTEN-ASD (n = 36; ASD + PTEN mutation), Macro-ASD (n = 25; ASD with macrocephaly, no PTEN mutation) and PTEN-no ASD (n = 23; no ASD, PTEN mutation). Sample size for recruitment in the ongoing study was determined based on pilot data which showed effect sizes (Cohen’s d) of 1.5–2.5, when comparing PTEN-ASD and Macro-ASD subjects.

PTEN Genotyping

Germline genomic DNA was extracted in the Genomic Medicine Institute’s Genomic Medicine Biorepository. PCR-based LightCycler mutation scanning and semi-automated PCR-based Sanger sequencing (ABI3730xl in Genomics Core Facility) of exons 1 through 9 and flanking intronic regions of genomic DNA was performed as per routine in the Eng lab since 1997 to reveal germline intragenic mutations in exons 1–9 and in splice sites. All novel variants were checked for presence and frequency in 350 ancestry-matched, sex-matched population controls (standard in Eng lab). PCR-based sequence analysis of the extended promoter region was also performed with Sanger sequencing to reveal promoter variants. All identified promoter variants were subjected to reporter assay as well as routine function interrogation. Multiplex ligation-dependent probe amplification was used to reveal large deletions and rearrangements.

Measures

All study participants completed a neuropsychological assessment that included cognitive and behavioral measures administered for research purposes. The cognitive battery included age-appropriate measures of global cognitive ability, attention/impulsivity, working memory, processing speed, language, and visuospatial skills. Due to anticipated severe symptom and intellectual impairment in at least a subset of cases, executive functions and motor coordination were not directly assessed, but rather inferred from parent/guardian ratings on standardized questionnaires. Parents/guardians also completed a number of inventories designed to measure autism symptoms, sensory processing, behavioral difficulties, and adaptive functioning. The specific cognitive and behavioral measures used in this study are outlined in Supplementary Table 1. All measures were scored according to published test manuals using age- and/or sex-corrected norms as available/appropriate. Higher scores on the cognitive measures generally reflect better cognitive performance, with the exception of the indices and subscales of the continuous performance test (CPT) and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF). In contrast, higher scores on the autism, sensory, and behavioral measures are typically indicative of greater symptom severity/behavioral difficulties, with the exception of the Short Sensory Profile (SSP).

Analyses

Baseline descriptive statistics stratified by diagnosis (PTEN-ASD, Macro-ASD, PTEN-no ASD) were calculated (Table 1). Statistics are presented as means with standard deviations for normally-distributed variables and medians with interquartile ranges for non-normal variables. Not all participants completed all study measures. Incomplete evaluations were largely due to the patient’s inability to tolerate testing or to time constraints. For those patients who were assessed, but who could not complete a particular measure due to low functional capacity, a score one point lower than the lowest possible score for that measure was assigned for purposes of data analyses. Assessments that were incomplete due to time constraints were considered missing at random.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of study groups

Chi-square analyses were used to examine differences in the proportion of missense versus truncating mutation types between PTEN-ASD and PTEN-no ASD.

The study groups were compared on cognitive and behavioral measures using the PTEN-ASD group as a reference. For normally-distributed variables, linear regression analysis was used. Residuals by fitted value plots were examined to verify homoscedasticity. Kruskal–Wallis tests with the Dwass, Steel, Critchlow-Fligner pairwise comparison tests were used to compare the non-normal variables. Due to the significant age difference among groups, the effect of age on each cognitive and behavioral measure was tested using Pearson or Spearman’s correlations where appropriate. If age was significantly associated with the measure, it was included as a control variable. Exploratory multiple regression analyses also examined IQ as a function of both group and measures of frontal lobe functioning, including processing speed and working memory.

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition (ADOS-2) was administered only to participants with ASD after diagnosis was confirmed by an expert clinical psychologist. PTEN-ASD and Macro-ASD were compared using the Wilcoxon rank-sum test.

One sample t-tests were used to compare mean performance of PTEN-no ASD to healthy control norms.

Given the exploratory nature of this study and relatively small sample sizes, we report effect sizes (Cohen’s d) in addition to p-values for all analyses rather than correcting for multiple comparisons. Effect sizes were interpreted as follows: small effect = 0.20, medium effect = 0.50, large effect = 0.80. Groups displayed homogeneity of variance for all comparisons. Analyses were two-sided and conducted using SAS Studio v. 3.3. SAS code is available upon request.

Results

Demographic characteristics

There were no significant differences in sex, race, or ethnicity among the three study groups. Macro-ASD was significantly older on average than both PTEN-ASD and PTEN-no ASD (p = 0.002) [Table 1].

Genetic analyses

A summary of the germline PTEN variants observed in the study cohort is provided in Supplementary Table 2. There was a similar proportion of missense mutations (52.8% vs. 34.8%, p = 0.176) and truncating variants (44.4% vs. 60.9%, p = 0.218) in PTEN-ASD compared to PTEN-no ASD.

Neurobehavioral profile of PTEN-ASD

PTEN-ASD demonstrated poorer performance than PTEN-no ASD in every cognitive domain assessed, with effect sizes for most measures in the medium to large range (i.e., Cohen’s d range 0.41–2.21). Caregivers of children in the PTEN-ASD group also observed greater symptoms of behavioral and sensory dysfunction than caregivers of children in the PTEN-no ASD group, with similar range effects (d range 0.49–2.06) (Supplementary Table 3 and Fig. 1).

Fig. 1: Group differences in cognitive and motor functioning.
figure 1

Higher scores indicate better cognitive performance for all domains, except attention, impulsivity, and reaction time. Full Scale IQ and processing speed are reported on a standard score scale (M = 100, SD = 15), attention, impulsivity, and reaction time are reported on a t-score scale (M = 50, SD = 10), and motor is reported as a raw score. The measures used to assess the noted cognitive domains are outlined in Supplementary Table 1. ASD, autism spectrum disorder

When compared to Macro-ASD, children with PTEN-ASD demonstrated greater cognitive impairments on measures of nonverbal intelligence (d = 0.65), attention (d= 0.73), reaction time (d = 0.85), and motor coordination (d = 0.69). While the threshold for statistical significance was not reached, medium to large effect sizes were also observed on measures of impulsivity (d = 0.61) and processing speed (d = 0.50) (Supplementary Table 4 and Fig. 1).

When controlling for working memory, the difference in FSIQ between PTEN-ASD and PTEN-no ASD decreased from 41.9 points to 6.3 points and the difference in FSIQ between PTEN-ASD and Macro-ASD decreased from 11.4 points to 5.5 points. Among subjects who completed the subtests that go into the Weschler Processing Speed Index, controlling for processing speed decreased the difference in FSIQ between PTEN-ASD and PTEN-no ASD from 46.7 points to 18.4 points and decreased the difference in FSIQ between PTEN-ASD and Macro-ASD from 19.3 points to 12.1 points.

PTEN-ASD had lower ADOS-2 Calibrated Severity Scores than Macro-ASD indicating less severe autism symptoms in this genetic sub-group (d = 0.56). While parents of both ASD groups noted less social responsiveness and more repetitive behavior than the PTEN-no ASD group, the two ASD groups did not significantly differ from each other on these scales (i.e., SRS and RBS-R) (Supplementary Tables 3 and 4). On caregiver-report questionnaires, PTEN-ASD were noted to have greater difficulties with sensory functioning than Macro-ASD (d = 0.62), particularly on the under-responsive/seeks sensation (d = 0.50), low energy/weak (d = 0.49), and taste/smell sensitivity (d = 0.50) scales (Supplementary Table 4 and Fig. 2). The two groups had similar symptoms on behavioral and adaptive behavior measures. (Supplementary Table 4).

Fig. 2: Group differences in sensory functioning.
figure 2

Higher scores indicate fewer sensory difficulties. Typical performance score ranges: Tactile Sensitivity 30–35, taste/smell sensitivity 15–20, under responsive/seeks sensation 27–35, low energy/weak 26–30. SSP, short sensory profile, ASD, autism spectrum disorder

Group performance on all cognitive and behavioral measures is summarized in Supplementary Tables 3 and 4, respectively, and a summary of observed neurobehavioral impairments by domain is provided in Table 2. It was necessary to control for age on CPT Commissions only.

Table 2 Summary of mean group performance on neurobehavioral measuresa by domain

Neurobehavioral profile of PTEN-no ASD

PTEN-no ASD did not significantly differ from established healthy control norms on measures of global cognitive ability. However, they had reduced mean scores compared to healthy controls on measures of subjective working memory (p = 0.011, d = 0.81), impulsivity (p = 0.002, d = 0.75), visuomotor integration (p = 0.012, d = 0.62), and motor coordination (p < 0.001, d = 1.48). In contrast, their scores on a measure of receptive vocabulary were significantly higher than established healthy controls norms (p = 0.021, d = 0.60) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3: Cognitive performance in PTEN-ASD compared to healthy control norms.
figure 3

Upgoing bars indicate performances higher than normative mean z-score of 0, and downgoing bars indicate performances lower than normative mean z-score of 0. BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, DCDQ, Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire, VABS, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale, ASD, autism spectrum disorder

On adaptive functioning measurements, PTEN-no ASD had reduced scores on the Motor Skills (p < 0.001, d = 0.95) and Internalizing (p = 0.035, d = 0.57) subscales of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Inventory—Second Edition compared to healthy control norms. Caregivers of children in the PTEN-no ASD group also reported more repetitive behaviors on the Repetitive Behavior Scale compared to healthy control norms (p = 0.004, d = 0.99). Interestingly, the mean scores of PTEN-no ASD were below normative expectation on the total score of the Short Sensory Profile suggesting greater sensory difficulties. Examination of the subtest scores revealed the most notable difficulties with low energy/weakness and auditory filtering.

Discussion

We have identified a distinct neuropsychological profile associated with mutations in PTEN suggesting primary disruption of frontal lobe systems21,22. Specifically, individuals with PTEN mutations showed reduced performance on measures of attention, impulsivity, reaction time, processing speed, and motor coordination, consistent with our pilot findings in independent patient cohorts19,20. This pattern of cognitive deficits may be related to neuroanatomical abnormalities. PTEN is a dual-specificity phosphatase that is a major inhibitor of the PI3K-AKT pathway23,24,25 with downstream effects on mTOR signaling, which helps regulate neural cell growth and proliferation26. Germline mutations in PTEN are associated with macrocephaly, with brain MRI showing markedly larger brain volumes as well as increased white matter hypointensities compared to other patient groups and healthy controls19,27,28. Even after controlling for total intracranial volume, volumetric differences remain between individuals with PTEN mutations and healthy controls in total white matter as well as in a number of specific brain regions, including the corpus callosum and cerebellum. While cortical thickness appears to vary across brain regions, our recent findings suggest reduced cortical thickness in the frontal regions of individuals with PTEN mutations19,28, which may account for our cognitive observations.

Ptenm3m4 murine model, a constitutional Pten knock-in model with autism-like phenotypes, demonstrate upregulated expression of genes implicated in myelination, including myelin basic protein and myelin proteolipid protein, possibly contributing to increased volumes of white matter structures19. Oligodendrocyte development relies on precise transcriptional control of genes involved in structural support of myelin29, and deregulation of this tightly controlled process may lead to white matter abnormalities. While behavioral studies in Ptenm3m4 mice have focused primarily on autism phenotypes (e.g., social interaction, repetitive behavior, anxiety)16, these mice do not appear to have global cognitive dysfunction and there is some evidence to suggest motor coordination deficits and social difficulties as observed in humans with PTEN mutations16. To our knowledge, attention, working memory and other more “frontal” type tasks have not yet been assessed in rodent models of Pten. Further research in this regard will be needed to validate our findings. It is also important to note that we cannot rule out the possibility that cerebellar dysfunction may contribute to this pattern given the known role of the cerebellum in many aspects of motor and cognitive functioning30,31,32.

While both PTEN groups showed cognitive patterns suggesting primary involvement of frontal lobe systems, PTEN-ASD had deficits that were more severe than PTEN-no ASD and extended to other areas of neurobehavioral function. Specifically, cognitive deficits in PTEN-no ASD tended to be mild and largely restricted to measures sensitive to frontal lobe function, whereas PTEN-ASD showed frontal deficits in the moderate to severe range accompanied by moderate to severe impairments in intellectual functioning and moderate deficits in both expressive and receptive language suggesting involvement of more widespread brain regions (Table 2). Interestingly, both PTEN groups reported deficits in sensory functioning, with the most notable problems on the under-responsive/seeks sensation, low energy/weak and taste/smell sensitivity subscales of the Short Sensory Profile (Fig. 3 and Supplementary Tables 3 and 4). The more severe neurobehavioral phenotype observed in PTEN-ASD may be related to more extensive brain abnormalities compared to PTEN-no ASD. While we are unaware of any studies that have directly compared neuroimaging characteristics of these two patient groups, anectodally PTEN-ASD appears to have greater white matter volume than PTEN-no ASD. This hypothesis remains to be confirmed in future studies with age-matched samples.

Similarly, while both ASD groups demonstrated rather global cognitive impairments, PTEN-ASD showed more severe impairments than those with Macro-ASD, particularly on measures associated with frontal systems dysfunction (e.g., attention, reaction time, motor function) (Supplementary Table 4). These findings are consistent with prior research in a small independent cohort suggesting reduced cognitive functioning in PTEN-ASD versus idiopathic ASD19 and likely reflect specific brain alterations including white matter abnormalities associated with PTEN mutations as previously discussed. In contrast, core ASD symptoms in PTEN-ASD appear to be similar to those seen in idiopathic ASD. PTEN-ASD and Macro-ASD reported similarities in social responsiveness and repetitive behavior (as assessed by the SRS and RBS, respectively); however, PTEN-ASD had lower clinical ratings of autism severity on the ADOS-2. This may reflect the passive/slower presentation of PTEN-ASD rather than a true reduction in autism severity. Indeed, caregiver responses on a sensory functioning measure suggest that PTEN-ASD often presents with low energy/weakness and under responsiveness. These findings are quite consistent with parent reports as well as our qualitative observations in the clinic.

Importantly, these study results suggest that the slow processing speed/reaction times, attention/working-memory difficulties, and motor deficits observed in individuals with PTEN mutations likely impact other aspects of their test performance. While mean FSIQ in PTEN-no ASD was within the average range (i.e., standard score of 99.2), scores on an untimed measure of receptive language (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test) known to be highly correlated with FSIQ were substantially higher (i.e., standard score 110.4, high average range). This suggests that FSIQ, which has attention/working memory, speed, and motor components, may underestimate general cognitive ability in PTEN patients. Indeed, our data would support this conclusion as differences in FSIQ observed between groups were much less apparent after adjusting for working memory and processing speed.

The rather consistent pattern of neurobehavioral deficits observed in individuals with PTEN muations is likely to have important clinical and educational implications, particularly for those diagnosed in childhood, and help contribute to our understanding of the underlying pathophysiology of autism symptoms and/or cognitive deficits in those with PTEN mutations. As such, comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation should be considered in children, adolescents, and young adults with PTEN mutations to thoroughly assess social and behavioral functioning as well as to identify cognitive strengths and weaknesses. Clinicians, and researchers, should also be sure to assess attention, speed and motor functions as part of any cognitive evaluation and to take those results into account when interpreting other test data and making clinical and educational recommendations. These type of assessments can inform tailored recommendations for academic accommodations and targeted interventions to optimize cognitive performance, adaptive functioning, and educational outcomes. For example, individuals with slow processing speed and/or attention/working memory difficulties may require information to be presented at a more deliberate pace and may benefit from brief delays in conversation to permit the formulation of their response. These individuals may also require academic accommodations to permit more time for assignments and tests, and educational expectations should be tempered. Tests that rely on motor coordination and speed should be avoided given the marked motor deficits associated with mutations in PTEN.

Taken together, results of this study suggest that children with PTEN-ASD have a distinct neurobehavioral phenotype and, as such, are likely to vary in several aspects of clinical presentation from patients with idiopathic ASD. Clinicians should make note of these cognitive and behavioral differences and be wary not to under-identify autism in individuals with PTEN mutations, as clinical observations may confound a relatively passive/slower processing phenotype with autism symptom severity. As in all individuals with autism, early identification is key to accessing evidence-based early interventions and maximizing developmental trajectories33,34,35. Comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation is recommended for the identification of cognitive strengths as well as weaknesses that may require remediation and/or accommodation. Similarly, the marked deficits in motor and sensory functioning that often accompany PTEN-ASD are likely to warrant thorough occupational and physical therapy evaluations for most patients. In contrast to idiopathic ASD, our results suggest that individuals with PTEN-ASD may require less in terms of assessment and/or intervention planning for challenging behavior and more with regard to cognitive rehabilitation/treatment. Ultimately, it is important for clinicians, parents, caregivers, and teachers to be made aware of these important phenotypic differences in order to obtain appropriate interventions and maximize day-to-day functioning.

There are a few limitations of the current study that deserve mention. There was a small subset of children in the study who were too low functioning to complete all cognitive measures. Rather than limit our dataset only to those with complete data and risk the potential of overestimating cognitive ability in this cohort, we assigned one lower than the lowest possible score for tasks that were attempted, but could not be completed. Further, while sample sizes in this cohort are among the highest reported to date, analyses for some measures were underpowered to detect medium effect sizes. In order to address this issue, we calculated effect sizes for each of the comparisons rather than rely solely on traditional p-values. It is also important to note that the Macro-ASD group was older than the two PTEN groups and, while not statistically significant, had a smaller proportion of females. Expanded recruitment in the overarching study is planned to achieve better demographic balance among groups and increase sample size, thereby improving power to detect smaller effects. Finally, while these cross-sectional data highlight some distinct neurobehavioral characteristics associated with PTEN-ASD, they give only a snapshot of functioning at a single time point in childhood/adolescence. Thus, efforts are currently underway to follow these children longitudinally and to extend recruitment to a broader age range (18 months to 45 years) in order to examine the evolution of neurobehavioral functioning in PTEN-ASD throughout development and into adulthood. Finally, future research will seek to better understand the mechanisms that drive the neurobehavioral differences we have observed. Our future work will examine relationships between neurobehavioral variables and underlying molecular and neurophysiological systems as well as neuroimaging measures. Equipped with this information, we hope to build a comprehensive longitudinal cross-level model that builds key links from PTEN mutations to neurobehavioral outcomes.